共同プロジェクト研究会 「物体の表面属性の視知覚に関わる脳内メカニズムの研究」
日時:平成25年12月25日(水),13:00~17:30(終了後,交流会),12月26日(木),10:00~16:00
場所:東北大学・電気通信研究所 2号館4階大会議室(W410号室)

色依存の静止画が動いて見える錯視

KITAOKA Akiyoshi (北岡 明佳) (Ritsumeikan University, Deptment of Psychology) email HP

since November 23, 2013  ハンドアウト


Fraser-Wilcox illusion group


「蛇の回転」

"Rotating snakes"

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 16)



"Rotating red disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise when this image is presented on a bright PC display or on a print with a bright environment.

Each disk appears to rotate conterclockwise when this image is presented on a dark PC display or on a print with a dark environment.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 17)


Conclusion in advance


"Moving snakes with a cloud of dust"

Snakes appear to move horizontally.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2009 (June 7)


"Moving arrows illusion"

Arrows appear to move.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (September 26)


"Blooming flowers"

Flowers appear to expand.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2008 (March 11)



「ぐいぐい昇竜」
Ascending dragon

竜が昇っていくように見える(下から見たところ)。
An dragon appears to rise.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2011 (February 12)


"Dongurakokko" (The donguri wave)

Donguri (acorns) appear to wave.

Copyright A.Kitaoka 2004 (April 19)



「トロの波」

図が波打って見える。

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2011 (September 5)



A brief history of phenomenal aspects of the Fraser-Wilcox illusion group

<Kitaoka, A. (forthcoming). The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension. In Shapiro, A. and Todorovic, D. (Eds.), Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions. Oxford University Press.>


1. Fraser and Wilcox (1979)

References

Fraser, A. and Wilcox, K. J. (1979). Perception of illusory movement. Nature, 281, 565-566.

Observers saw illusory motion in a stationary image which consists of repeated luminnace gradient in a saw-tooth wave form. Some reported illusory motion from dark to light along a luminance gradient, while others saw the reversal. The illusion is strong in the peripheral viewing. Fraser and Wilcox (1979) claimed that this individual difference depends on some genetic properties.



Adopted from “Alex Fraser, Geneticist and Painter”
(http://doctoralexfraser.blogspot.jp/p/spirals.html) <access August 25, 2012> (with permission from Alan Fraser)

Alex Fraser (1923-2002) was a geneticist and a painter.



2. Faubert and Herbert (1999)

The left disk appears to rotate clockwise while the right one counterclockwise. Faubert and Herbert (1999) reported only illusory motion from dark to light along a luminance gradient. They attributed the illusory motion to the assumed time difference between responses to black and white.

References

Faubert, J. and Herbert, A. M. (1999). The peripheral drift illusion: A motion illusion in the visual periphery. Perception, 28, 617-621.



3. Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000)

The left disk appears to rotate clockwise while the right one counterclockwise. Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000), too, reported only illusory motion from dark to light along a luminance gradient. They revealed that the illusion magnitude is a positive, nearly linear function of contrast.

References

Naor-Raz, G. and Sekuler, R. (2000). Perceptual dimorphism in visual motion from stationary patterns. Perception, 29, 325-335.


This disk appears to rotate clockwise.

This color illusion image presented by Naor-Raz and Sekuler (2000) (shown below) might be attributable to this type.
http://www.perceptionweb.com/perception/perc0300/sekuler.jpg <access August 26, 2012>
<with permission from Robert Sekuler>


4. Kitaoka and Ashida (2003)

The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise. Kitaoka and Ashida (2003) proposed that the illusory motion is strong when patterns are arranged in the following luminance order: from black, dark-gray, white, light-gray, and back to black.

References

Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion. VISION (Journal of the Vision Society of Japan), 15, 261-262. PDF



5. Kitaoka (2007)



high-resolution file

Kitaoka (2007) classified the Fraser-Wilcox illusion into Type I, IIa, IIb and III, in each of which the dark-to-light type was distinguished from the light-to-dark one.

References

Kitaoka, A. (2007) Phenomenal classification of the “optimized” Fraser-Wilcox illusion and the effect of color. Poster presentation in DemoNight, VSS2007, GWiz, Sarasota, Florida, USA, May 14, 2007.



Type I


"Dark to light"

The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.



"Light to dark"

The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.


"Dark to light" and "light to dark" combined
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.


"Dark to light" and "light to dark" combined, with color enhancement
The left disk appears to rotate counterclockwise while the right one clockwise.


The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.

Type I (six disks) 1500 x 1000 (pixel)

Type I (six disks) 6000 x 4000 (pixel)


Type II

Type IIa

The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.


Type IIb

The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.


Type IIa (six disks) 1500 x 1007 (pixel)

Type IIa (six disks) 6000 x 4031 (pixel)


Type IIb (six disks) 1500 x 1000 (pixel)

Type IIb (six disks) 6000 x 4000 (pixel)




"Rotating snakes"
(Ver. 2011)

Each disk appears to rotate.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2011 (January 23)

Adjusted for printing using EPSON PM-4000PX



Type III


The upper row appears to move rightward while the lower one leftward.


Type III (two rows) 1500 x 400 (pixel)

Type III (two rows) 4500 x 1200 (pixel)Type III  4500 x 1200


The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle disks appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.



6. Kitaoka (2008a, b)

"Type IV" was added to the list.

References

Kitaoka, A. (2008a). Optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusions: a pictorial classification by Akiyoshi Kitaoka. Talk in Workshop No.005 "Experimental studies of anomalous motion illusions" in the 72nd Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, September 19, 2008. Talk

Kitaoka, A. (2008b). A new type of the optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion in a 3D-like 2D image with highlight or shade. Journal of Three Dimensional Images (Japan), 22(4), 31-32. PDF PDF (manuscript) Presentation

Type IV


The upper-left, upper-right, and lower-middle rings appear to rotate counterclockwise while the rest clockwise.


Type dark to light light to dark Examples
IV
new!










Findings of the "Rotating snakes" illusion (optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion)
1. Illusion is strong in the peripheral vision
Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2003) Phenomenal characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion. VISION, 15, 261-262.PDF
2. Illusion is strong when the image is of high luminance
Hisakata, R. and Murakami, I. (2008) The effects of eccentricity and retinal illuminance on the illusory motion seen in a stationary luminance gradient. Vision Research, 49, 1940-1948.
3. Illusion requires some eye movement
Murakami, I., Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2006) A positive correlation between fixation instability and the strength of illusory motion in a static display. Vision Research, 46, 2421-2431.
Kuriki, I., Ashida, H., Murakami, I., and Kitaoka, A. (2008) Functional brain imaging of the Rotating Snakes illusion by fMRI. Journal of Vision, 8(10):16, 1-10.
4. Illusion does not occur in the stabilized retinal image
Murakami, I., Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2006) A positive correlation between fixation instability and the strength of illusory motion in a static display. Vision Research, 46, 2421-2431.
5. Illusion is enhanced by color
Kitaoka, A. (2007) Phenomenal classification of the “optimized” Fraser-Wilcox illusion and the effect of color. Poster presentation in DemoNight, VSS2007, GWiz, Sarasota, Florida, USA, May 14, 2007.
6. Illusion is weak when the image is moved
Tomimatsu, E., Ito, H., Seno, T., and Sunaga, S. (2010) The ‘rotating snakes’ in smooth motion do not appear to rotate. Perception, 39, 721-724.
7. Illusion is weak when the image is observed through pinholes
Cantor, C. R. L., Tahir, H. J., and Shor, C. M. (2010) Is the rotating snakes an optical illusion? VSS2010, #26.434.
8. Illusion activates V1 through hMT+ areas
Kuriki, I., Ashida, H., Murakami, I., and Kitaoka, A. (2008) Functional brain imaging of the Rotating Snakes illusion by fMRI. Journal of Vision, 8(10):16, 1-10.
Ashida, H., Kuriki, I., Murakami, I., Hisakata, R. and Kitaoka, A. (2012). Direction-specific fMRI adaptation reveals the visual cortical network underlying the "Rotating Snakes" illusion. NeuroImage, 61, 1143–1152.

Major models of the "Rotating snakes" illusion (optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion)
1. Adaptation (temporal positional shift)
Backus, B. T. and Oruç, I. (2005) Illusory motion from change over time in the response to contrast and luminance. Journal of Vision, 5, 1055-1069.
2. Visual delay (temporal positional shift) related issue 1 _ related issue 2
Conway, R. B., Kitaoka, A., Yazdanbakhsh, A., Pack, C. C., and Livingstone, M. S. (2005) Neural basis for a powerful static motion illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5651-5656.
3. Motion signal asymmetry
Murakami, I., Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2006) A positive correlation between fixation instability and the strength of illusory motion in a static display. Vision Research, 46, 2421-2431.

7. Kitaoka (2008, 2010, 2012a, b)

A strongly color-dependent, "reddish" version was added to the list.

In 2008, Kitaoka found that the direction of illusory motion is “red → dark purple → purple → magenta (light red-purple) → red”, and that the illusion magnitude is strong when overall luminance contrast is low. Images of high luminance make a strong effect.

"Rotating red fruits"

Rings of fruits appear to rotate.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2008 (December 19)

Purple » light purple » light red-purple » red » purple

References

Kitaoka, A. (2010). Introduction to visual illusion. Tokyo: Asakura-shoten (in Japanese). Book

Kitaoka, A. (2012a). Designs using the red Fraser-Wilcox illusion. Journal of the Color Science Association of Japan, 36, Supplement, 188-189. PDF (color manuscript) Talk

Kitaoka, A. (2012b) The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension. Perception 41 ECVP Abstract Supplement, page 91 (Poster September 3, 2012). Poster PDF



8. Kitaoka (2011)

In 2011, Kitaoka found that flickering the image increases the illusion magnitude. Blinks also work.



Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2012 (June 14)


Further studies on the flickering enhancement or induction (Kitaoka, 2013)

Flickering induction depends on the interaction between the stimulus and the blank.

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise during fading-in while counterclockwise during fading-out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 19)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (August 20)


Each disk appears to rotate clockwise during fading-in while counterclockwise during fading-out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 19)


Each disk appears to rotate counteclockwise during fading-in while clockwise during fading-out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 19)


Each disk appears to rotate counteclockwise during fading-in while clockwise during fading-out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 19)


Each disk appears to expand during fading-in while to contract during fading-out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 20)



"On-off rotating snakes: red and sky blue #1"

When fading in, each disk appears to rotate in the direction: black, red, white, sky blue, black. The direction is reversed when fading out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 30)



"On-off rotating snakes: red and sky blue #2"

When fading in, each disk appears to rotate in the direction: black, sky blue, white, red, black. The direction is reversed when fading out.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (April 30)


9. Yanaka and Hirano (2011)

Yanaka and Hilano (2011) reported that this illusion is enhanced by mechanical shaking.

Each disk appears to rotate counterclockwise.
(Note that this movie is not their original)

References

Yanaka, K. and Hilano, T. (2011). Mechanical shaking system to enhance "Optimized Fraser–Wilcox Illusion Type V". Perception, 40, ECVP Abstract Supplement, page 171.



10. Yanaka (2012)

Yanaka (2012) reported that this illusion is enhanced by viewing images under dark illumination.

References

Yanaka, K. (2012). Enhancement of the optimized Fraser-Wilcox illusion Type V by swinging the image. Talk in the 5th Illusion Workshop, Meiji University, Tokyo, Japan, September 18, 2012.


11. Kitaoka (2013)

In 2013, Kitaoka (ECVP2013) found that Yanaka’s darkening enhancement is accompanied by a reversal of illusion: i.e., the direction under dark condition is “red → magenta (light red-purple) → purple → dark purple → red”.

References

Kitaoka, A. (2013). Invisible color and visible color: Mystery of color the brain feels. Talk in the Shizuoka Science Museum Ru-Ku-Ru, Shizuoka, Japan, January 13, 2013. Talk


12. Kitaoka (2013)

In 2013, Kitaoka (ECVP2013) found that combination of a long wavelength color and a short one makes the illusions.


Illusory motion depending on the combination of colors



"Rotating red-and-purple disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)



"Rotating red-and-cyan disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 1)



"Rotating red-and-cyan disks 2"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)



"Rotating red-and-blue disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 29)



"Rotating red-and-green disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (January 29)



"Rotating blue-and-green disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it slightly appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)



"Rotating blue-and-yellow disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 6)



"Rotating blue-and-yellow disks 2"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 12)



"Rotating red-and-yellow disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise on a bright display, while it appears to rotate counterclockwise with a printed image under dark illumination.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 23)



"Rotating yellow-and-green disks"

Each disk appears to rotate clockwise or counterclockwise on a bright display.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 23)



13. Kitaoka (2013)

In 2013, Kitaoka (ECVP2013) found that increasing or decreasing illumination makes the illusions.



14. Kitaoka (2013)

It is suggested that this ‘luminance change-induced’ motion* might generate the color-dependent Fraser-Wilcox illusion, being triggered by saccades, blinks, flickering or shaking the images.

*The luminance change-induced’ motion includes the reverse phi phenomenon (Anstis, 1970; Anstis and Rogers, 1975) and the phenomenal phenomena (Gregory and Heard, 1983). Directions of illusory motion depending on the changes of luminance are shown in the right (Kitaoka, 2006).

References

Anstis S M, 1970 “Phi movement as a subtraction process” Vision Research 10 1411-1430
Anstis S M, Rogers B J, 1975 “Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast” Vision Research 15 957-961
Gregory R L, Heard P F, 1983 “Visual dissociations of movement, position, and stereo depth: some phenomenal phenomena” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35A 217-237
Kitaoka, A. (2006). Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception, 35, 799-806. animations --- PDF


"Rotating gray-scale disks (movie)"

Disks appear to rotate clockwise or counterclockwise.

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (February 15)


輝度変調誘導性の運動錯視
luminance change-induced motion illusion


Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment to van Lier and Csathó (2006)

Kitaoka, A. (2006). Configurational coincidence among six phenomena: A comment on van Lier and Csathó (2006). Perception, 35, 799-806. animations --- PDF

Adobe Flash Player is necessary to play animations on this page.


Animation 1

Reversed phi movement (Anstis and Rogers 1975). There are four rectangles, each of which is flanked by thin lines that are dark or light. When the luminance of the rectangles decreases and that of the background increases accordingly, the upper two appear to converge in motion while the lower two appear to go apart in motion. On the other hand, when the luminance of the rectangles increases and that of the background decreases accordingly, the upper two appear to go apart in motion while the lower two appear to approach each other in motion. In sum, the direction of apparent motion is from the dark flank to the neighboring part that goes darkening or from the light flank to the neighboring part that goes brightening. Although the rectangles are aligned vertically, the apparent positional displacement occurs in the opposite direction to motion. If observers cross-fuse (uncross-fuse) the right and left rectangles, the upper rectangle appears to be in front of (behind) the lower one when they are dark.

Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 2

The illusory motion investigated by Gregory and Heard (1983). This appearance is quite similar to Animation 1. The only configurational difference from the reversed phi movement is that the luminance of the rectangles is constant.

Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 3

The illusory motion mentioned by Gregory and Heard (1983), in which the luminance of the rectangles dynamically changes while that of the surround is constant. This appearance is quite similar to Animations 1 and 2.

Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 4

Phi movement (Anstis and Rogers 1975). For panel (a), dark stationary rectangles are placed on a bright stationary background. When the luminance of the right flank of each rectangle increases and that of the left flank decreases, the rectangle appears to shift leftward in motion as well as in position. Conversely, the luminance of the right flank decreases and that of the left flank increases, the rectangle appears to shift rightward in motion and position. If observers cross-fuse (uncross-fuse) the right and left rectangles, the fused rectangle appears to be in front (behind) when the right flank of the right rectangle are dark and the left flank of the left rectangle are dark. For panel (b), bright rectangles are placed on a dark background. In this case, phenomena are the reversal of panel (a).

(a)      (b)


(a) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(b) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 5

Consistency between the reversed phi movement and stereopsis (Anstis and Rogers 1975). In this animation, there are two phases in motion, in which the upper-left and the lower-right rectangles are one eighth phase in advance of the upper-right and the lower-left rectangles. For example, when the luminance of the rectangles increases, the upper-left rectangle appear to go leftward followed by the upper-right one, while the lower-right rectangle appear to go leftward followed by the lower-left one. In this period, the cross-fused (uncross-fused) upper rectangle appears to be in front (behind) as compared with the cross-fused (uncross-fused) lower rectangle.

Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 6

Consistency between the apparent motion and Café-Wall-like tilt illusions (van Lier and Csathó 2005). According to dynamic changes in luminance gradient, the flanks of the rectangle appears to tilt dynamically. (a) The luminance gradient given to both flanks is dynamically changed while the dark-gray rectangle and the light-gray background are stationary. The dark part of the gradient appears to shift outward in motion and position while the bright part appears to shift inward in motion and position. This distortion agrees with the tilt illusion. (b) The luminance gradient given to the rectangle is dynamically changed while the dark flanks and the gray background are stationary. At the dark part of the gradient, the neighboring part of flanks appears to shift inward in motion and position, while at the bright part of the gradient, the neighboring part of flanks appears to shift outward in motion and position. This distortion agrees with the tilt illusion.

(a)        (b)

(a) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(b) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 7

Inconsistency between the apparent motion and Café-Wall-like tilt illusions. According to dynamic changes in homogenous areas, the flanks of the rectangle appears to tilt dynamically. (a) The luminance of the homogeneous rectangle is dynamically changed while the flanks with luminance gradient and the gray background are stationary. The darkening rectangle appears to attract the dark parts of the flanks inward in motion while the brightening rectangles appears to attract the light parts of the flanks inward in motion. This distortion disagrees with the tilt illusion. (b) The luminance of the homogeneous flanks is dynamically changed while the rectangle with luminance gradient and the gray background are stationary. At the dark part of the gradient, brightening flanks appears to shift inward in motion, while at the bright part of the gradient, darkening flanks appears to shift inward in motion. This distortion disagrees with the tilt illusion

(a)        (b)

(a) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(b) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Animation 8

Demonstration of the apparent motion in the three elemental spatio-temporal configurations (Figure 6). The behaviors in (a), (b) and (g) are summarized in Figure 6a (off-center line-type configuration); those in (c), (d) and (h) are in Figure 6b (on-center line-type configuration); and those in (e), (f) and (i) are summarized in Figure 6c (edge-type configuration). For further explanations, see the text.

(a)        (b)

(a) Flash(swf) --- Quicklime(mov)
(b) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)

(c)         (d)

(c) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(d) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)

(e)         (f)

(e) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(f) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)

(g)        (h)        (i)

(g) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(h) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)
(i) Flash(swf) --- QuickTime(mov)


Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2010 (April 3)


Nice demos

Arthur Shapiro's Illusions and Demonstration


Directions of illusory motion depending on changes of luminance in each area






Involvement of the Purkinje shift is suggested. Short wavelength color appears to be relatively bright under a dark condition. Decreasing illumination makes short wavelength color relatively bright. This might induce the luminance change-dependent motion illusion and show the color-dependent Fraser-Wilcox illusion.


References

Kanazawa, S., Kitaoka, A., and Yamaguchi, M. K. (2013). Infants see illusory motion in static figures. Perception, 42, 828-834.

Ashida, H., Kuriki, I., Murakami, I., Hisakata, R. and Kitaoka, A. (2012). Direction-specific fMRI adaptation reveals the visual cortical network underlying the "Rotating Snakes" illusion. NeuroImage, 61, 1143–1152.

Otero-Millan, J.,Macknik, S. L., and Martinez-Conde, S. (2012). Microsaccades and Blinks Trigger Illusory Rotation in the “Rotating Snakes” Illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6043-6051.

Stevanov, J., Marković, S., and Kitaoka, A. (2012). Aesthetic valence of visual illusions. i-Perception, 3(2), 112–140.

Tomimatsu E., Ito, H., Sunaga, S., & Remijn, G. B. (2011). Halt and recovery of illusory motion perception from peripherally viewed static images. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1823-1832.

Idesawa, M. (2010). A Model of Illusory Motion Perception in Still Figures. Optical Review, 17, 557–561.

Tomimatsu, E., Ito, H., Seno, T., and Sunaga, S. (2010). The ‘rotating snakes’ in smooth motion do not appear to rotate. Perception, 39, 721-724.

Fermüller, C., Ji, H., and Kitaoka, A. (2010). Illusory motion due to causal time filtering. Vision Research, 50, 315–329.

Billino, J., Hamburger, K., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2009). Age effects on the perception of motion illusions. Perception, 38, 508-521.

Kuriki, I., Ashida, H., Murakami, I., and Kitaoka, A. (2008). Functional brain imaging of the Rotating Snakes illusion by fMRI. Journal of Vision, 8(10):16, 1-10.

Hisakata, R. and Murakami, I. (2008) The effects of eccentricity and retinal illuminance on the illusory motion seen in a stationary luminance gradient. Vision Research, 49, 1940-1948.

Beer, A. L., Heckel, A. H. and Greenlee, M. W. (2008) A motion illusion reveals mechanisms of perceptual stabilization. PLoS ONE, 3(7): e2741, 1-7. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002741

Chi, M-T., Lee, T-Y., Qu, Y., and Wong, T-T. (2008) Self-Animating Images: Illusory Motion Using Repeated Asymmetric Patterns. ACM Transaction on Graphics (Proceedings of SIGGRAPH 2008), 27, No.3.

Murakami, I., Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2006) A positive correlation between fixation instability and the strength of illusory motion in a static display. Vision Research, 46, 2421-2431.

Conway, R. B., Kitaoka, A., Yazdanbakhsh, A., Pack, C. C., and Livingstone, M. S. (2005) Neural basis for a powerful static motion illusion. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5651-5656.

Backus, B. T. and Oruç, I. (2005) Illusory motion from change over time in the response to contrast and luminance. Journal of Vision, 5, 1055-1069.

Kitaoka, A. and Ashida, H. (2003) Phenomenal characteristics of the peripheral drift illusion. VISION (Journal of the Vision Society of Japan), 15, 261-262. PDF

Naor-Raz, G. and Sekuler, R. (2000) Perceptual dimorphism in visual motion from stationary patterns. Perception, 29, 325-335.

Faubert, J. and Herbert, A. M. (1999) The peripheral drift illusion: A motion illusion in the visual periphery. Perception, 28, 617-621.

Fraser, A. and Wilcox, K. J. (1979) Perception of illusory movement. Nature, 281, 565-566.


色変調誘導性の運動錯視を発見!(2013年12月24日)
Discovery of the color change-induced motion illusion! (December 24, 2013)


90 x 90 pixel

180 x 180 pixel

270 x 270 pixel

360 x 360 pixel

540 x 540 pixel

Flash(swf)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)



Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)


90 x 90 pixel

180 x 180 pixel

270 x 270 pixel

Flash(swf)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)


90 x 90 pixel

180 x 180 pixel

270 x 270 pixel

Flash(swf)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)


90 x 90 pixel

180 x 180 pixel

270 x 270 pixel

Flash(swf)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)

Copyright Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2013 (December 24)


red --- yellow --- green --- blue



"Introduction to visual illusions"



ご清聴ありがとうございました。


Akiyoshi's illusion pages